Phases/pitch (Alexander Galembo )


Subject: Phases/pitch
From:    Alexander Galembo  <galembo(at)PSYC.QUEENSU.CA>
Date:    Mon, 9 Feb 1998 15:58:05 -0500

Dear List - I have got couple of messages asking to post the responses I have got to my quiery about the influence of phases on pitch. Attached are the responses except those sent directky to the Auditory list, so everybody knows them. Alexander Galembo, Ph. D. NATO Science fellow Acoustics lab, Dept. of Psychology, Queen's University Kingston ON K7L3N6 Canada Tel. (613) 5456000, ext. 5754 Fax (613) 5452499 E-mail: galembo(at)pavlov.psyc.queensu.ca URL : http://www.geocities.com/CapeCanaveral/Lab/8779/ ================ From: bil(at)ccrma.Stanford.EDU (Bill Schottstaedt) Date: Wed, 4 Feb 98 07:45:45 -0800 To: Alexander Galembo <galembo(at)psyc.queensu.ca> Subject: Re: Pitch of a complex tone I think John Pierce has written a couple papers on that subject: Pierce, J. R. (1990). Rate, place, and pitch with tonebursts. Music Perception , 7(3):205-212. Pierce, J. R. (1991a ). Periodicity and pitch perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 90:1889-1893. ================ From: "R. Parncutt" <psa03(at)cc.keele.ac.uk> Subject: Re: Pitch of a complex tone To: galembo(at)psyc.queensu.ca Date: Wed, 4 Feb 1998 16:10:45 +0000 (GMT) > It is classical that the pitch of a periodic complex tone is independent on > phases of harmonics. > > I would appreciate to be informed about any publications providng a doubt > in this phase independence (if exist). How about... Langner, G. (1997). Temporal processing of pitch in the auditory system. Journal of New Music Research, 26, 116-132. Langner, G., & Schreiner, C.E. (1988). Periodicity coding in the inferior colliculus of the cat (I) - Neuronal mechanisms. Journal of Neurophysiology, 60, 1799-1822. Meddis, R., & Hewitt, M.J. (1991a). Virtual pitch and phase sensitivity of a computer model of the auditory periphery. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 2866-2894. Patterson, R.D. (1973). The effects of relative phase and the number of components on residue pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 53, 1565-1572. That reminds me! I'd be grateful for brief, private, independent assessment of Langner's work. _____________________________________________________________________ Richard Parncutt, Lecturer in Psychology of Music and Psychoacoustics, Unit for the Study of Musical Skill and Development, Keele University. Post: Dept of Psychology, Keele University, Staffordshire ST5 5BG, GB. Tel: 01782 583392 (w),01782 719747 (h). Email: r.parncutt(at)keele.ac.uk. Fax: +44 1782 583387. URL: http://www.keele.ac.uk/depts/ps/rpbiog.htm. ============== Date: Wed, 4 Feb 1998 16:21:33 GMT To: Alexander Galembo <galembo(at)psyc.queensu.ca> From: bob.carlyon(at)mrc-apu.cam.ac.uk (Bob Carlyon) Subject: Re: Pitch of a complex tone Prof. Galembo, Trevor Shackleton and I did some experiments showing that, compared to a sine-phase stimulus, and alternating-phase stimulus has a pitch about 1 octave higher, provided that its harmonics are unresolved by teh peripheral auditory system. We also review earlier papers showing similar pitch-doubling effects. The article is published in jasa vol 95 p3529-3540 (1994) regards bob carlyon ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- Dr. Bob Carlyon MRC Applied Psychology Unit 15 Chaucer Rd. CAMBRIDGE CB2 2EF England Phone: (44) 1223 355294 ext 720 FAX: (44) 1223 359062 email: bob.carlyon(at)mrc-apu.cam.ac.uk ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ================ Date: Wed, 4 Feb 1998 08:49:59 -0800 From: Malcolm Slaney <malcolm(at)INTERVAL.COM> Subject: Re: Pitch of a complex tone To: Alexander Galembo <galembo(at)PSYC.QUEENSU.CA>, At 6:28 AM -0800 2/4/98, Alexander Galembo wrote: >I would appreciate to be informed about any publications providng a doubt >in this phase independence (if exist). I think the first were Flanagon and Guttman (1960). I'm not sure if they described their observations as a phase change, but they are. Pierce, a few years ago, redid the experiments. A description of this stimulai that Richard Duda wrote for the Apple Hearing Demo Reel is appended to the end of my note. -- Malcolm This animation was produced in conjunction with Richard Duda of the Department of Electrical Engineering at San Jose State University during the Summer of 1989. Thanks to Richard Duda for both the audio examples and the explanation that follows and to John Pierce for calling this experiment to our attention. Researchers in psychoacoustics have long looked to cochlear models to explain the perception of musical pitch [Small70]. Many experiments have made it clear that the auditory system has more than one mechanism for pitch estimation. In one of these experiments, Flanagan and Guttman used short-duration impulse trains to investigate two different mechanisms for matching periodic sounds, one based on spectrum and one based on pulse rate [Flanagan60]. They used two different impulse trains, one having one pulse per period of the fundamental, the other having four pulses per period, every fourth pulse being negative . These signals have the interesting property that they have the same power spectrum, which seems to suggest that they should have the same pitch. The standard conclusion, however, was that below 150 pulses per second the trains "matched" if they had the same pulse rate; they "matched" on spectrum only when the fundamental frequency was above about 200 Hz. [Pierce89] modified this experiment by replacing the pulses by tone bursts=F3short periods of a 4,800-Hz sine wave modulated by a raised-cosine Hamming window. In essence, he used Flanagan and Guttman's pulses to amplitude modulate a steady high-frequency carrier. His purpose in doing this was to narrow the spectrum, keeping the large response of the basilar membrane near one place (the 4,800-Hz place), regardless of pulse rate. To be more specific, Pierce used the three signal "patterns" shown below. All have the same burst duration, which is one-eighth of a pattern period. Pattern a has four bursts in a pattern period. Pattern b has the same burst rate or pulse rate, but every fourth burst is inverted in phase. Thus, the fundamental frequency of b is a factor of four or two octaves lower than that of a. Pattern c has only one burst per pattern period, and thus has the same period as b; in fact, it can be shown that b and c have the same power spectrum. Thus, a and b sound alike at low pulse rates where pulse-rate is dominant, and b and c sound alike at high pulse rates where spectrum is dominant. Pierce observed that the ear matches a and b for pattern frequencies below 75 Hz, and matches b and c for pattern frequencies above 300 Hz. He found the interval between 75 and 300 Hz to be ambiguous, the b pattern being described as sounding inharmonic. Pierce's tone bursts. Patterns a and b have the same pulse rate frequency, while b and c have the same power spectrum. Here the test sounds are shown with one cycle per burst. To see if and how these results are reflected in correlograms, a similar set of tone burst signals were generated. The only difference between our signals and Pierce's signals was due to differences in the digital sampling rate used. To get a Fourier spectrum with minimum spectral splatter, Pierce imposed two requirements: 1)The tone-burst frequency fb was set at half the Nyquist rate. Where Pierce's 19,200-Hz sampling rate led to fb =3D 4,800 Hz, our 16,000-Hz sampling rate forced fb down to 4,000 Hz. 2)Each burst had to contain an exact integral number n of cycles. This number, n, is a major parameter for the experiments, ranging from 1 to 128. If the pattern period is T, then to obtain exactly n cycles of frequency fb in time T/8 requires that fb T/8 =3D n, so that T =3D 8n/fb . Thus, to obtain the same spectral characteristics, we had to use different numerical values for the tone-burst frequency fb and the corresponding pattern period T. The table shown below is our version of Table I in Pierce's paper. A set of eight test signals was generated according to this scheme. Each test signal consists of a sequence of the a, b and c patterns, each pattern lasting 1.024 seconds. This time interval was chosen to get an exact integer number of bursts, ranging from 4 for Case 1c to 2000 for Cases 8a and 8b. ============ Alexander Galembo, Ph. D. NATO Science fellow Acoustics lab, Dept. of Psychology, Queen's University Kingston ON K7L3N6 Canada Tel. (613) 5456000, ext. 5754 Fax (613) 5452499 E-mail: galembo(at)pavlov.psyc.queensu.ca URL : http://www.geocities.com/CapeCanaveral/Lab/8779/


This message came from the mail archive
http://www.auditory.org/postings/1998/
maintained by:
DAn Ellis <dpwe@ee.columbia.edu>
Electrical Engineering Dept., Columbia University